Attitude formation
when an
attitude forms because it has been reinforced through reward or a pleasant
experience or discouraged through punishment or an unpleasant experience. For
example, a parent might praise a teenager for helping out at an after-school
program with little kids. As a result, the teen may develop a positive attitude
toward volunteer work. Similarly, many people find that broccoli has a terrible
taste, and so they dislike broccoli because of its punishing flavor.
Why measure attitudes
People have likes and dislikes and have them in varying degrees.
But why study and measure them? Attitudes are action tendencies and as such
they can facilitate or hinder action at all levels--individual, group,
community, state, and national.
Consider the case of population control. Among the various
possible activities toward progress, the government of India wanted its people
to adopt birth control and compulsory sterilization was introduced in certain
parts of the country. But, this move, as we all know, gave rise to immense anxiety
and antagonism among people and the programme ended in a fiasco.
Such blatant disregard for people's attitudes not only thwarted
many a corresponding programme of the government, but, tagged on to other
grievances of the kind under Emergency, also turned into a massive time bomb
which, after an adequate gestation period, blew up the once invincible reign of
Mrs. Gandhi and her colleagues.
At the same time, certain other activities like the introduction
of new variety seeds in agriculture and the introduction of adult education had
a better fate.
To determine, therefore, what action to introduce when and how to
introduce it for the desired effect among a target population, the action
planners must know how far the existing action tendencies of the population are
receptive/resistant to the proposed action.
Such knowledge would help devise appropriate means for triggering
the desired change.
Approaches to Attitude
Measurement
How do we measure attitudes? We have said that we arrive at
measures of attitudes by inference. But we need data on which to base our
inference. Such data are collected by various methods.
We may observe the ongoing behavior of people in the natural setting;
we may directly ask the respondents to state their feelings with
regard to the issue under study;
we may assign a well-defined task to respondents and record their
performance;
we could generate data by giving the respondents partially
structured stimuli to interpret or react to.
Direct observation
This method involves recording the actual behavior of people whose
attitude is to be studied. It is indeed an objective method and well suited for
certain kinds of issues. For example, it is quite commendable to observe the
actual overt behavior of strikers by participating in the strike itself to gain
a measure of the strikers' attitude. We could also observe a company executive
in his day-to-day dealings with his subordinates to assess his attitudes towards
them. Not all issues, however, lend themselves to direct observation. Can you,
for instance, use this method of data collection to study the attitude of
voters? Even the most dedicated and non-partisan psychologist would not have
access to the polling booths to observe the actual voting behavior of people.
Further, even when we have managed to spot some behavior related
to our study, we do not know if the behavior was an outcome of the related
attitude or one caused by other factors. Take for instance a boy who goes to
church regularly. You have observed his behavior, all right. But, does this behavior
mean that he is favorably inclined toward prayer and religion? Not necessarily.
On asking him directly, you may discover that his girl friend cherishes religious
sentiments and attends prayer services regularly. He goes there only to meet
her!
Direct questioning
If we want to know how people feel about a certain thing, it seems
most natural to ask them straight away as to what their feelings are. But,
however logical and smooth this technique may seem to be, it serves only a
limited purpose of roughly classifying respondents as favorable, unfavorable,
and indifferent with regard to a psychological object.
if you were to study
people's attitudes toward the National Emergency when the Emergency was on,
many a respondent would be reluctant to give you an answer or would, in all
likelihood, loudly proclaim a favorable attitude. When controversial issues are
involved and pressures and threats are operative, direct questioning is not the
suitable means of data collection for assessing attitudes.
Even when no threats are present, not all individuals are capable
of articulating their feelings. A person may possess certain attitudes and
behave accordingly, but may not be aware of them.
Scales of Measurement
Measurement is assignment of mathematical symbols to objects and
events according to rules. In order to assign different symbols to different
objects, one must be able to differentiate objects on a given aspect, attribute
or property. Such differentiation may be rough and crude or may be refined and
specific. You may, for instance, want just to classify objects, persons, or
responses into different categories. A nominal scale will
suffice for this purpose. The only criterion to assign "objects" to
different categories of a nominal scale is whether the objects are the same or
different with regard to the property being studied. To classify individuals,
for example, according to the province they come from or according to the
religion they belong to would constitute a nominal scale. If you assign numbers
to the different categories in this scale, the numbers are just identification
names. They are not amenable to mathematical operations like calculations of
means, coefficients of correlation, etc. You can, of course, count the number
of subjects under each category label (numeral or verbal) and find the modal
category in which the highest number of individuals fall. You may also perform
a test of association, if you categorized the individuals according to two (or
more) attributes. For example, if you categorized individuals both according to
their province and their religion, you could perform the Chi-square test to see
if a particular province(s) tend(s) to be associated with a particular
religion.
If you want to know the relative positions of persons or objects
with respect to a characteristic, you need an ordinal scale, in
which individuals or objects are ranked as first, second,
third, etc., depending on the more or less of the attribute
possessed by the individuals or objects. The ordinal scale can state who has
more or less of the attribute under study, but not how much more or how much
less. If person P is ranked first, Q second, R third, etc., we cannot know if
the difference between P and Q is or is not the same as the difference between,
for instance, U and V; the magnitude of difference between any two consecutive
ranks remains unknown and is likely to vary.
An interval scale can tell us whether P is as
much higher than Q as Y is than Z on a particular attribute. In other words, in
an interval scale, the difference between any two adjacent positions is the
same as the one between any other two adjacent positions. Thus, the interval
scale is an improvement over the ordinal scale, even as the latter is over the
nominal scale.
There is another type of scale, called the ratio scale,
which is commonly used in the physical sciences. To have a ratio scale the
absolute zero point needs to be determined. A ten-inch rod can be said to be
exactly twice as long as a five-inch one, because both the rods share a common
starting point, namely, the real zero point. But in the subject matter of the
social sciences, the zero point is arbitrary and, therefore, we cannot express
relationships in terms of strict ratios. Psychophysics has made attempts, in
limited areas, to establish absolute zero points. By and large, however, social
sciences do not use ratio scales; they employ ordinal and interval scales in
their studies.
With a view to assessing the degree of attitudes possessed by
persons and to be able to study a large number of people, the scaling technique
was introduced into attitude measurement. Various scales of attitude
measurement have been developed. Here we shall only broadly discuss the
characteristics of some prevalent attitude scales so as to get acquainted with
the general steps involved in their construction and use. It is likely that, in
spite of numerous scales being available(*), you do not find one handy or
suitable when you take up a particular study. Knowledge of how to develop an
attitude scale will obviate such a crippling situation and help you have an
instrument tailor-made for a given study. For a detailed discussion of how to
construct an attitude scale, you may refer to Allen L. Edwards' (1957)
Techniques of Attitude Scale Construction.
Thurstone's Scale
To construct the Thurstone scale, a large number of statements are
collected which express various possible opinions about the issue or object of
study. These statements, after an editing for relevance and clarity, are sorted
into eleven sets that ranges from most unfavorable, through neutral,
to most favorable.
When administered, the respondent just checks the items s/he
agrees with and her/his attitude score is the mean value of the items s/he
checked.
Likert's Scale
For the Likert scale, various opinion statements are collected,
edited and then given to a group of subjects to rate the statements on a
five-point continuum: 1=strongly agree; 2=agree; 3=undecided; 4=disagree; and
5=strongly disagree. The subjects express the degree (one to five) of their personal
agreement or disagreement with each of the statements. Only those items which
in the analysis best differentiate the high scorers and the low scorers of the
sample subjects are retained and the scale is ready for use. To measure the
attitude of a given group of respondents, this scale is given to them and every
respondent indicates whether s/he strongly agrees, agrees,
is undecided, disagrees, or strongly disagrees with
each statement. The respondent's attitude score is the sum of her/his ratings
of all the statements. For this reason, the Likert scale is also known as the
scale of Summated Ratings.
In the Thurstone scale, the respondent checks only those items
with which s/he agrees, whereas in the Likert scale s/he indicates her/his
degree of agreement or disagreement for all the items in the scale.
By and large, a great majority of researchers prefer the Likert
technique to Thurstone's
0 comments
Post a Comment
thanks